当前位置:主页 > 教育论文 > 阅读论文 >

阅读研究中的主要眼动指标评述 南京廖华

发布时间:2017-01-06 08:49

  本文关键词:阅读研究中的主要眼动指标评述,,由笔耕文化传播整理发布。


第4期 闫国利等: 阅读研究中的主要眼动指标评述 599

5 眼动指标使用时应注意的问题

5.1 眼动指标的分类

以上对阅读中的眼动指标进行了介绍。需要说明的是, 对眼动指标进行分类的标准并不是唯一的, 还可以根据其他的标准进行分类, 比如根据研究目的的不同还可以划分为局部分析指标(local measures)和整体分析指标(global measures) (Bai, Yan, Zang, Liversedge, & Rayner, 2008; Li, Zhao, & Pollatsek, 2012)。整体分析指标主要是从宏观上对阅读的眼动特征进行分析, 具体包括:平均注视时间、平均眼跳距离、向前眼跳次数、回视次数(比率)、总的句子阅读时间(在句子阅读过程中落在句子上的所有注视点和眼跳的总和)、句子阅读时间校正(指总的句子阅读时间减去眼跳时间, 即落在句子上所有注视点的持续时间的总和)、注视位置等(沈德立等, 2010)。具体对一个兴趣区的深入分析主要使用局部分析指标, 主要包括:单一注视时间、首次注视时间、第一遍阅读时间、向前通过阅读时间、第二次通过阅读时间、总阅读时间、总体通过阅读时间、重读时间、注视位置、再注视比率、跳读率、回视出比率等。当然这种分类也只是相对的, 比如注视位置、眼跳距离等既可以作为整体分析指标也可以在局部分析中使用。

5.2 原始数据的删除标准

在正式分析眼动数据之前, 还需要对有问题的原始数据进行剔除。

首先, 要删去眼动记录丢失较多的试验项目, 同时还要删去包含有眨眼的注视, 因为有研究(Ridder & Tomlinson, 1995)发现, 与眼跳抑制一样, 在眨眼时也存在“眨眼抑制” (blink suppression)现象。根据Volkmann, Riggs和Moore (1980)的研究, 眨眼抑制从眨眼启动前约50~100ms开始, 一直持续到眨眼结束后的100~150 ms, 最近的一项研究(Ehrmann, Ho, & Paoas, 2005)甚至发现要持续到眨眼结束后的200~500 ms。

其次, 要删去注视和眼跳的极端数据。注视时眼睛并不是稳定不动, 而是有微小的跳动, 眼跳距离只有一个字符的很小部分, 这种微小的眼跳(microsaccade)可能表示注意力的调整, 也可能是反映了眼动系统内在的校正倾向。关于阅读中微小眼跳的功能至今没有一种合理的解释, 它与

知觉和认知加工之间的关系目前也没有统一的认识(Engbert, 2006; Engbert & Kliegl, 2003; Martinez- Conde, Macknik, Troncoso, & Hubel, 2009)。因此在数据分析之前, 研究者会参考以往研究选定一个最小眼跳距离的标准, 凡是小于该标准的微小眼跳将被视为眼动记录时的“噪音”, 在数据分析时被删去, 或者看作是较大眼跳的一部分而被合并 (Inhoff et al.,1998)。从眼跳距离的频率分布中可以看出微小眼跳的概率极小(见图4), 同样大于15~20个字符空间(4°~5°视角)的眼跳也较少, 因此在有的研究中, 大于15~20个字符空间的眼跳也被作为极端数据删除。

同样对于短暂注视的功能目前也没有合理的解释, 多数研究者认为短暂注视并不受即时的认知加工过程控制, 阅读者在短暂的注视期间并不能获得有效信息(Rayner & Pollatsek, 1989; Inhoff, Briihl & Schwarz,1996), 因此在数据分析之前需要删去短暂注视。关于短暂注视的界定标准也有一定差异, 早期有研究者将50 ms以下的注视界定为短暂注视 (Inhoff et al., 1996), 由图4可见, 小于100 ms的注视在眼动记录中概率很小, 因此也有研究将标准限定在70~100 ms(Vitu, O’Regan, & Mittau, 1990)。当前国内外阅读的眼动研究中一般都采用80 ms这一标准 (Reingold, Yang, & Rayner, 2010; Slattery, Angele, & Rayner, 2011; Chen & Huang, 2012; van Gompela, Pickering, Pearson, & Liversedge, 2005; 白学军等, 2011; 陈庆荣等, 2010; 任桂琴, 韩玉昌, 于泽, 2012)。也有研究者认为中文阅读中, 似乎短注视点相对英文更多, 因此将标准定为60ms (Wang, Chen, Yang, & Mo, 2008; Yang, Wang,Tong, & Rayner, 2012; 王穂苹, 佟秀红, 杨锦绵, 冷英, 2009)。如果小于删除标准的注视与之前或之后的注视处在一个字符空间内, 就将这两个注视时间合并作为一次注视, 否则就将短暂注视删去。需要说明的是, 如果有的研究在计算注视时间时将眼跳时间包含在内, 那么短暂注视的界定标准就要更长, 而且, 当对语言加工过程进行研究时, 相对较高的界定标准可能更有利, 因为有研究发现, 140 ms及其短于140 ms的注视不受阅读材料的词汇特征的影响(McConkie, Reddix, & Zola, 1992)。时间在1000 ms以上的注视点几乎不存在(见图4), 如果在眼动数据中出现1000 ms以上的注视点, 极有可能

600

心 理 科 学 进 展 第21卷

图4 注视时间和向前眼跳距离的频率分布(来自Rayner, 1998) 注:注视时间以毫秒为单位, 眼跳距离以字符空间为单位。

是仪器误差导致, 因此也被作为极端数据删除。另外, 也有研究将平均注视时间的两个或三个标准差作为删除标准(Rayner et al., 2010), 大于和小于两个或三个标准差的注视时间和眼跳距离作为极端数据删除。

需要说明的是, 不管是眼跳距离还是注视时间, 在选择极端数据的删除标准时, 研究者需要特别谨慎, 因为不同的删除标准将对数据的最终解释效果产生较大影响, 比如, 在有的研究数据中, 注视时间的删除标准为50 ms时, 最终数据的效应值可能不显著, 但是如果删除标准界定为140 ms时效应值可能会非常显著。目前在阅读的眼动研究中仍无统一的删除标准。 5.3 眼动指标的选择

如何针对不同的研究目的, 选取适当而有效的眼动指标并对其进行分析是眼动分析法的关键。选择合适的眼动指标, 要根据研究内容和研究目的而定。同时如上文所讲每一个眼动指标都有其适用的范围和优缺点, 因此在研究中只使用某一个或某一类指标是有局限性的, 应该综合使用多种眼动指标, 从不同维度对数据进行细致分析。

陈庆荣, 谭顶良, 邓铸, 徐晓东. (2010). 句法预测对句子理解影响的眼动实验. 心理学报, 42(6), 672–682. 蒋波, 章菁华. (2011). 1980-2009年国内眼动研究的文献计量分析. 心理科学, 34(1), 235–239.

韩玉昌, 隋雪, 任延涛. (2005). 小学学习困难生阅读过程中的眼动特征. 心理科学, 28(3), 550–553.

任桂琴, 韩玉昌, 于泽. (2012). 句子语境中汉语词汇形音作用的眼动研究. 心理学报, 44(4), 427–434.

沈德立, 白学军, 臧传丽, 闫国利, 冯本才, 范晓红. (2010). 词切分对初学者句子阅读影响的眼动研究. 心

理学报, 42(2), 159–172.

王穂苹, 佟秀红, 杨锦绵, 冷英. (2009). 中文句子阅读中语义信息对眼动预视效应的影响. 心理学报, 141(3), 220–232.

熊建萍, 闫国利, 白学军. (2009). 不同年级学生汉语阅读知觉广度的眼动研究. 心理科学, 32(3), 584–587. 闫国利, 白学军. (2000). 中文阅读过程的眼动研究. 心理

学动态, 8(3), 19–22.

闫国利, 白学军. (2007). 汉语阅读的眼动研究. 心理与行

为研究, 5(3), 222–234.

闫国利, 王丽红, 巫金根, 白学军. (2011). 不同年级学生阅读知觉广度及预视效益的眼动研究. 心理学报, 43(3), 249–263.

张仙峰, 叶文玲. (2006). 当前阅读研究中眼动指标述评.

心理与行为研究, 4(3), 236–240.

Abrams, R. A., Meyer, D. E., & Kornblum, S. (1989). Speed and accuracy of saccadic eye movements: Characteristics of impluse variability in the oculomotor system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 529–543.

参考文献

白学军, 孟红霞, 王敬欣, 田静, 臧传丽, 闫国利. (2011). 阅读障碍儿童与其年龄和能力匹配儿童阅读空格文本的注视位置效应. 心理学报, 43(8), 851–862.

第4期 闫国利等: 阅读研究中的主要眼动指标评述 601

Altmann, G. T. M., Garnham, A., & Dennis, Y. (1992). Avoiding the garden path: Eye movements in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 685–712.

Ashby, J. (2006). Prosody in skilled silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 318–333.

Ashby, J., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2005). The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements during silent reading. Cognition, 96, 89–100.

Ashby, J., Rayner, K., & Clifton, C. (2005). Eye movements of highly skilled and average readers: Differential effects of frequency and predictability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1065–1086.

Bai, X J, Yan, G. L, Liversedge, S. P., Zang, C. L., & Rayner, K. (2008). Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1277–1287.

Becker, W., & Jürgens, R. (1979). An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Research, 19, 967–983.

Binder, K. S., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1999). Extraction of information to the left of the fixated word in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1162–1172.

Blythe, H. I., H?iki?, T., Bertam, R., Liversedge, S. P., & Hy?n?, J. (2011). Reading disappearing text: Why do children refixate words? Vision Research, 51, 84–92. Brysbaert, M., Drieghe, D., & Vitu, F. (2005). Word skipping: Implications for theories of eye movement control in reading. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Cognitive processes in eye guidance (pp. 53–77). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Calvo, M. G., Meseguer, E., & Carreiras, M. (2001). Inferences about predictable events: Eye movements during reading. Psychological Research, 65, 158–169. Chen, Q. R., & Huang, Y. (2012). Processing coordinate structures in Chinese: Evidence from eye movements. PLoS ONE, 7(4), e35517.

Clifton, C., Staub, J. A., & Rayner, K. (2007). Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In R. Van Gompel, M. Ficsher, W. S. Murray, & R. L. Hill (Eds.), Eye movements: A window on mind and brain (pp. 341–372). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Clifton, C., Bock, J., & Radó, J. (2000). Effects of the focus particle only and intrinsic contrast on comprehension of reduced relative clauses. In A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller, & J. Pynte (Eds.), Reading as a Perceptual Process (pp. 591–619). Oxford: Elsevier.

Clifton, C., Traxler, M. J., Mohamed, M. T., Williams, R. S.,

Morris, R. K., & Rayner, K. (2003). The use of thematic role information in parsing: Syntactic processing autonomy revisited. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 317–334.

Drieghe, D., Brysbaert, M., Desmet, T., & De Baecke, G. (2004). Word skipping in reading: On the interplay of linguistic and visual factors. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 79–103.

Drieghe, D., Desmet, T., & Brysbaert, M. (2007). How important are linguistic factors in word skipping during reading? British Journal of Psychology, 98, 157–171. Drieghe, D., Pollatsek, A., Staub, A., & Rayner, K. (2008). The word grouping hypothesis and eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1552–1560.

Drieghe, D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2008). Mislocated fixations can account for parafoveal-on-foveal effects in eye movements during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(8), 1239–1249.

Ehrmann, K., Ho, A., & Papas, E. (2005). A novel method for assessing variations in visual acuity after the blink. Contact Lens and Anterior Eye, 28(4), 157–162.

Engbert, R. (2006). Microsaccades: A microcosm for research on oculomotor control, attention, and visual perception. Progress in Brain Research, 154, 177–192. Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research, 43(9), 1035–1045.

Frisson, S., Rayner, K., & Pickering, M. J. (2005). Effects of contextual predictability and transitional probability on eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 862–877.

Gautier, V., O’Regan, J. K., & LaGargasson, J. F. (2000). “The skipping” revisited in French: Programming saccades to skip the article “les”. Vision Research, 40, 2517–2531.

H?iki?, T., Bertram, R., Hy?n?, J., & Neimi, P. (2009). Development of the letter identity span in reading: Evidence from the eye movement moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 167–181.

Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F. (1990). Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: Implications for attention and eye movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16(3), 417–429.

Holmqvist, K., Nystr?m, N., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Jarodzka, H., & Van de Weijer, J. (2011). Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures. Oxford,

602 心 理 科 学 进 展 第21卷

UK: Oxford University Press.

Huang Y. T., & Gordon, P. C. (2011). Distinguishing the time course of lexical and discourse processes through context, coreference, and quantified expressions, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(4), 966–978.

Hy?n?, J., Bertram, R., & Pollatsek, A. (2004). Are long compound words identified serially via their constituents? Evidence from an eyemovement-contingent display Irwin, D. E., & Brockmole, J. R. (2004). Suppressing where but not what: The effect of saccades on dorsal- and ventral-stream visual processing. Psychological Science, 15(7), 467–473.

Irwin, D. E., & Carlson-Radvansky, L. A. (1996). Cognitive suppression during saccadic eye movements. Psychological Science, 7, 83–88.

Juhasz, B. J., White, S. J., Liversedge, S. P., & Rayner, K. (2008). Eye movements and the use of parafoveal word change study. Memory & Cognition, 32(4), 523–532. Hy?n?, J., Lorch Jr, R. F., & Kaakinen, J. (2002). Individual differences in reading to summarize expository text: Evidence from eye fixation patterns. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 44–55.

Hy?n?, J., Lorch Jr, R. F., & Rinck, M. (2003). Eye movement measures to study global text processing. In J. Hy?n?, R. Radach, & H. Deubel (Eds.), The mind's eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research (pp. 159–176). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.

Hy?n?, J., & Niemi, P. (1990). Eye movements during repeated reading of a text. Acta Psychologica, 73, 259–280.

Hy?n?, J., Tommola, J., & Alaja, A. M. (1995). Pupil dilation as a measure of processing load in simultaneous interpretation and other language. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 48(3), 598–612.

Inhoff, A. W., Briihl, D., & Schwarz, J. (1996). Compound word effects differ in reading, on-line naming, and delayed naming tasks. Memory & Cognition, 24, 466–476. Inhoff, A. W., & Liu, W. M. (1998). The perceptual span and oculomotor activity during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 20–34.

Inhoff, A. W., & Radach, R. (1998). Definition and computation of oculomotor measures in the study of cognitive processes. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 29–53). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Inhoff, A., Eiter, B., Radach, R., & Juhasz, B. (2003). Distinct subsystems for the parafoveal processing of spatial and linguistic information during eye fixations in reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 803–828.

Inhoff, A. W., & Weger, U. W. (2005). Memory for word location during reading: Eye movements to previously read words are spatially selective but not precise. Memory & Cognition, 33, 447–461.

Irwin, D. E. (1998). Lexical processing during saccadic eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 1–27.

length information in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1560–1579.

Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1993). The intensity dimension of thought: Pupillometric indices of sentence processing. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(2), 310–339.

Kliegl, R., & Engbert, R. (2005). Fixation durations before word skipping in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 132–138.

Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M., & Engbert, R. (2004). Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 262–284.

Kliegl, R., Risse, S., & Laubrock, J. (2007). Preview benefit and parafoveal-on-foveal effects from word n+2. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1250–1255.

Krügel, A., & Engbert, R. (2010). On the launch-site effect for skipped words during reading, Vision Research, 50, 1532–1539.

Kuchinke, L., V?, M. L. -H., Hofmann, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2007). Pupillary responses during lexical decisions vary with word frequency but not emotional valence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 65, 132–140. Li, X. S., Liu P. P., & Rayner, K. (2011). Eye movement guidance in Chinese reading: Is there a preferred viewing location? Vision Research, 51, 1146–1156.

Li, X., Zhao, W., & Pollatsek, A. (2012). Dividing lines at the word boundary position helps reading in Chinese. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(5), 929–934. Liversedge, S. P., Paterson, K. B., & Pickering, M. J. (1998). Eye movements and measures of reading time. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 55–77). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L., Troncoso, X. G., & Hubel, D. H. (2009). Microsaccades: A neurophysiological analysis. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(9), 463–475. Matin, E. (1974). Saccadic suppression: A review and an analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 899–917.

McConkie, G. W., Reddix, M. R., & Zola, D. (1992).


  本文关键词:阅读研究中的主要眼动指标评述,由笔耕文化传播整理发布。



本文编号:235807

资料下载
论文发表

本文链接:https://www.wllwen.com/jiaoyulunwen/ydsp/235807.html


Copyright(c)文论论文网All Rights Reserved | 网站地图 |

版权申明:资料由用户3c14d***提供,本站仅收录摘要或目录,作者需要删除请E-mail邮箱bigeng88@qq.com