特质焦虑个体的注意偏向和注意控制特点及其认知神经机制研究
[Abstract]:AIM: Trait anxiety tends to induce individual attention bias toward threatening stimuli, and this negative attention bias plays a central role in the occurrence and development of anxiety and anxiety disorders. In addition, the relationship between trait anxiety and attention bias may be regulated by attention control. Therefore, attention bias and attention control of trait anxiety individuals become the focus of this study. Three groups of subjects, low trait anxiety (LTA), high trait anxiety (HTA) and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), were tested in order to clarify the different application scope of anxiety neurocognitive model and attentional control theory, and to clarify the individual trait anxiety at different levels. Methods: (1) Using the emotional space-cue attention paradigm, the stimulus spacing between cues and stimuli was set at 50 ms and 800 ms to investigate the characteristics of attention bias and its temporal variation in different levels of trait anxiety, and to analyze resting state EEG. The asymmetry of alpha waves in the prefrontal region reveals the underlying neural mechanisms. (2) Study 2 and Study 3 examined the characteristics of distraction inhibition and conflict adaptation in normal subjects with high, low trait anxiety and clinical anxiety disorder under emotional and non-emotional Flanker tasks, and paid attention to Bishop's anxiety neurocognitive model and Eysenck's attention. Control theory was validated. (3) In study 4, single-task emotional Flanker test and two-task emotional Flanker test were used to investigate the attention-switching function and potential cognitive neural mechanism of trait anxiety individuals at different levels. There was no significant difference between LTA group and HTA group (p = 0.898) in response time between p0.001 and HTA group (381.24 (+ 12.92), but there was no significant difference between LTA group and HTA group (p = 0.898). The results of resting state EEG showed that anxiety could significantly predict the asymmetry of alpha waves in the prefrontal region under depression control; the differences between groups showed that GAD patients were significantly higher than LTA, while HTA patients had a lower degree of alpha wave lateralization, i.e. a lower activation level in the left cerebral region. Flanker EEG study found that the response time of GAD group was longer than LTA group (632.98+19.81, p0.001) and HTA group (655.11+17.71, p0.001). The error rate of GAD group (2.51+0.40) was higher than LTA group (1.40+0.31, p=0.038) and HTA group (1.37+0.31, p=0.027). In inconsistent trials, compared with LTA group and HTA group, the N2 latency of GAD group was higher. Compared with LTA group, P3 amplitude of HTA group and GAD group was significantly lower. Both HTA group and GAD group showed poor conflict adaptation in behavior and N2 components. (3) Non-emotional Flanker EEG test showed that the response time of GAD group (619.69 1) There was no significant difference between HTA group and LTA group (p = 0.945), but there was no significant difference between LTA group and HTA group (p = 0.001) and between LTA group and HTA group (p = 0.001). The latency of N2 component (265.89 +5.45) was longer in LTA group (243.70 +5.45, P = 0.005, P = 0.005) and HTA group (252.54 +4.78, P = 0.070) than in LTA group (243.70 +5.45, P = 0.45, P = 0.005) and HTA group (252.54 +4.54 +4.78, P = 0.78, P = 0.070). The amplitudeof P3 (4.94 [(4.94 +0.86) in GAD group was significantly lower than that in LTA group (9.10 [(9.10 +0.86, P = 0.86, P = 0.86, P = 0.001 Responses of the GAD group to the task The error rate of GAD group was significantly higher than that of LTA group (3.05 + 0.52, P 0.001) and HTA group (3.05 + 0.52, P 0.001) and LTA group (719.88 + 127.87, P 0.001) and HTA group (719.88 + 127.87, P 0.001) and HTA group (736.76 [106.76 [106.72, P 0.72, P 0.001, P 0.001); the error rate of GAD group (5.38 [(5.38 [(0.63) was significantly higher than LTA group (3.38.38 [(3.38 [(3.38 [0.38] [0.63) significantly higher than LTA group (3.05 [(3.HTA group ( There was no significant difference in EEG parameters among the three groups. (5) In the emotional space-cue task, the response time variability of GAD patients was significantly higher than that of LTA patients (3.07 0.62, P = 0.012) and HTA patients (2.96 0.63, P = 0.001), but there was no significant difference between LTA and HTA patients (p = 0.487). Similar results were obtained in the R test, the non-emotional Flanker test and the dual-task emotional Flanker test. Conclusion: (1) In the late attention processing stage, the subjects with high trait anxiety had attention fixation on negative stimuli, and the patients with anxiety disorder had attention avoidance. Low is closely related to negative attention avoidance. (2) In clinical GAD patients, both emotional and non-emotional stimuli show poor conflict adaptation, low attention inhibition and switching ability, and insufficient top-down attention control. High trait anxiety and low attention control may be the underlying causes of clinical anxiety disorder. Neurocognitive model can better explain the attention characteristics of patients with clinical anxiety disorder, while attention control theory is more suitable for normal people with high trait anxiety. Variability is an important indicator of attention control, which can better distinguish the patients with clinical anxiety disorder from the normal population, and has the stability of cross-experimental paradigm and experimental materials.
【学位授予单位】:第三军医大学
【学位级别】:博士
【学位授予年份】:2017
【分类号】:B842
【相似文献】
相关期刊论文 前10条
1 黄文强;;对特质焦虑理论的初步研究[J];中小企业管理与科技(上旬刊);2009年02期
2 徐芳芳;;高特质焦虑的注意偏向研究[J];牡丹江教育学院学报;2010年05期
3 海倩;张俊丽;;特质焦虑影响因素研究综述[J];才智;2010年30期
4 黄辛隐;任艳超;;特质焦虑对面孔情绪识别的影响[J];心理技术与应用;2013年03期
5 肖凌燕,邹泓;大学生特质焦虑:结构及其特点[J];心理发展与教育;2000年04期
6 李焰,张世桐;中学生特质焦虑的发展[J];健康心理学杂志;2000年01期
7 李焰,张世彤,王极盛;中学生特质焦虑与其影响因素的模型建构[J];心理学报;2002年03期
8 黄建,于志涛;大学生特质焦虑的影响因素研究[J];广州体育学院学报;2005年01期
9 柳春香;黄希庭;;特质焦虑大学生注意偏向的实验研究[J];心理科学;2008年06期
10 刘溪;梁宝勇;;心算应激与特质焦虑、应对风格的关系[J];心理与行为研究;2008年01期
相关会议论文 前10条
1 张晓龙;陈英和;;研究生的特质焦虑、抑郁、主动控制特点及关系[A];第十一届全国心理学学术会议论文摘要集[C];2007年
2 郭小朝;李良明;丁亚平;朱召烈;;空军飞行(学)员特质焦虑水平调查结果的初步分析[A];第一届全国人—机—环境系统工程学术会议论文集[C];1993年
3 李雪燕;辛涛;;特质焦虑的潜类别回归分析[A];第十一届全国心理学学术会议论文摘要集[C];2007年
4 柳春香;黄希庭;任泽港;;特质焦虑大学生注意偏向的实验研究[A];第十一届全国心理学学术会议论文摘要集[C];2007年
5 周路平;孔令明;;特质焦虑、性别差异对风险回避的影响[A];第十二届全国心理学学术大会论文摘要集[C];2009年
6 陈建民;黄恩;徐晓青;张载福;吕望强;;职业中专学生的特质焦虑与父母教养方式、人格特征关系[A];2005年浙江省精神病学学术年会论文汇编[C];2005年
7 黄鹏;杨志兵;苗丹民;路惠捷;朱霞;;不同特质焦虑大学生在内隐联想测验中的表现[A];增强心理学服务社会的意识和功能——中国心理学会成立90周年纪念大会暨第十四届全国心理学学术会议论文摘要集[C];2011年
8 敖立功;;对大学生状态-特质焦虑问题的调查与分析[A];中国心理卫生协会大学生心理咨询专业委员会全国第七届大学生心理健康教育与心理咨询学术交流会暨专业委员会成立十周年纪念大会论文集[C];2001年
9 葛君慧;崔天雪;严荣燕;沈如璋;陈硕卒;赵梦岩;张禹;;音乐对不同焦虑水平者注意偏向的影响[A];心理学与创新能力提升——第十六届全国心理学学术会议论文集[C];2013年
10 王兰爽;;音乐、腹式呼吸对高特质焦虑女大学生的心率变异性影响[A];心理学与创新能力提升——第十六届全国心理学学术会议论文集[C];2013年
相关博士学位论文 前5条
1 于永菊;特质焦虑个体的注意偏向和注意控制特点及其认知神经机制研究[D];第三军医大学;2017年
2 齐森青;特质焦虑影响抑制控制的认知神经机制[D];西南大学;2014年
3 顾艳;Go/Nogo任务下“冷”、“热”执行功能对数字、名词材料的抑制加工[D];苏州大学;2013年
4 邵秀巧;特质焦虑者对威胁信息注意偏向的实验研究[D];天津师范大学;2008年
5 贾丽萍;情绪对不同焦虑水平个体抑制功能的影响[D];天津师范大学;2014年
相关硕士学位论文 前10条
1 冯庆林;大学生特质焦虑影响因素的结构模型及其与特质焦虑关系的模型建构[D];云南师范大学;2003年
2 黄阳;特质焦虑对面孔情绪识别的影响研究[D];苏州大学;2012年
3 许明明;高特质焦虑者对人际评价信息的注意偏向[D];郑州大学;2015年
4 傅青青;不同难度下中央执行功能在特质焦虑与估算表现中的中介效应研究[D];苏州大学;2015年
5 罗秋惠;民间信仰与焦虑的外显和内隐关系研究[D];闽南师范大学;2015年
6 王富;不同情绪线索下特质焦虑大学生的重复知盲效应研究[D];苏州大学;2015年
7 陈琴;特质焦虑大学生的负性注意偏向及其时间进程特点[D];陕西师范大学;2015年
8 邢泽宇;想象接触下不同特质焦虑水平大学生对艾滋群体内隐偏见的影响[D];江西师范大学;2015年
9 李清瑶;瑜伽锻炼对女性练习者特质焦虑情绪影响的研究[D];西安体育学院;2015年
10 袁晓明;特质焦虑对决策的影响:期望的作用机制[D];内蒙古师范大学;2015年
,本文编号:2238814
本文链接:https://www.wllwen.com/shekelunwen/xinlixingwei/2238814.html